Piano Guidance
Photo by MART  PRODUCTION Pexels Logo Photo: MART PRODUCTION

What is the IQ of a genius style?

Historical IQ classification tables IQ Range ("ratio IQ") IQ Classification Above 140 "Near" genius or genius 120–140 Very superior intelligence 110–120 Superior intelligence 90–110 Normal, or average, intelligence 3 more rows

How long is a lesson plan?
How long is a lesson plan?

two pages It outlines what students will learn during each class period, how the lesson will be taught and how student progress will be measured....

Read More »
What is the most important chord in music?
What is the most important chord in music?

I, IV and V I, IV and V are the basic building blocks for chord progressions in western music. You'll find these chords playing an important role...

Read More »

Categorisation of people based on IQ

Score distribution chart for sample of 905 children tested on 1916 Stanford–Binet Test IQ classification is the practice by Intelligence quotient (IQ) test publishers of labeling IQ score ranges with category names such as "superior" or "average".[1][2][3][4] The current scoring method for all IQ tests is the "deviation IQ". In this method, an IQ score of 100 means that the test-taker's performance on the test is at the median level of performance in the sample of test-takers of about the same age as was used to norm the test. An IQ score of 115 means performance one standard deviation above the median, a score of 85 performance, one standard deviation below the median, and so on.[5] Deviation IQs are now used for standard scoring of all IQ tests in large part because they allow a consistent definition of IQ for both children and adults. By the current "deviation IQ" definition of IQ test standard scores, about two-thirds of all test-takers obtain scores from 85 to 115, and about 5 percent of the population scores above 125.[6] Lewis Terman and other early developers of IQ tests noticed that most child IQ scores come out to approximately the same number regardless of testing procedure. All IQ tests show a slight variation in scores even when the same person takes the same test over and over again.[7][8] IQ scores also slightly differ for a test-taker taking tests from more than one publisher at the same age.[9] The various test publishers do not use uniform names or definitions for IQ score classifications. Even before IQ tests were invented, there were attempts to classify people into intelligence categories by observing their behavior in daily life.[10][11] Those other forms of behavioral observation are still important for validating classifications based primarily on IQ test scores. Both intelligence classification by observation of behavior outside the testing room and classification by IQ testing depend on the definition of "intelligence" used in a particular case and on the reliability and error of estimation in the classification procedure.

Differences in individual IQ classification [ edit ]

IQ scores can differ to some degree for the same person on different IQ tests, so a person does not always belong to the same IQ score range each time the person is tested. (IQ score table data and pupil pseudonyms adapted from description of KABC-II norming study cited in Kaufman 2009.[12][13]) Pupil KABC-II WISC-III WJ-III Asher 90 95 111 Brianna 125 110 105 Colin 100 93 101 Danica 116 127 118 Elpha 93 105 93 Fritz 106 105 105 Georgi 95 100 90 Hector 112 113 103 Imelda 104 96 97 Jose 101 99 86 Keoku 81 78 75 Leo 116 124 102 IQ tests generally are reliable enough that most people 10 years of age and older have similar IQ scores throughout life.[14] Still, some individuals score very differently when taking the same test at different times or when taking more than one kind of IQ test at the same age.[15] For example, many children in the famous longitudinal Genetic Studies of Genius begun in 1921 by Lewis Terman showed declines in IQ as they grew up. Terman recruited school pupils based on referrals from teachers, and gave them his Stanford–Binet IQ test. Children with an IQ above 140 by that test were included in the study. There were 643 children in the main study group. When the students who could be contacted again (503 students) were retested at high school age, they were found to have dropped 9 IQ points on average in Stanford–Binet IQ. More than two dozen children dropped by 15 IQ points and six by 25 points or more. Yet parents of those children thought that the children were still as bright as ever, or even brighter.[16] Because all IQ tests have error of measurement in the test-taker's IQ score, a test-giver should always inform the test-taker of the confidence interval around the score obtained on a given occasion of taking each test.[17] IQ scores are ordinal scores and are not expressed in an interval measurement unit.[18][19][20][21][22] Besides the inherent error band around any IQ test score because tests are a "sample racks of learned behavior", IQ scores can also be misleading because test-givers fail to follow standardized administration and scoring procedures. In cases of test-giver mistakes, the usual result is that tests are scored too leniently, giving the test-taker a higher IQ score than the test-taker's performance justifies. Some test-givers err by showing a "halo effect", with low-IQ individuals receiving IQ scores even lower than if standardized procedures were followed, while high-IQ individuals receive inflated IQ scores.[23] IQ classifications for individuals also vary because category labels for IQ score ranges are specific to each brand of test. The test publishers do not have a uniform practice of labeling IQ score ranges, nor do they have a consistent practice of dividing up IQ score ranges into categories of the same size or with the same boundary scores.[24] Thus psychologists should specify which test was given when reporting a test-taker's IQ.[25] Psychologists and IQ test authors recommend that psychologists adopt the terminology of each test publisher when reporting IQ score ranges.[26][27] IQ classifications from IQ testing are not the last word on how a test-taker will do in life, nor are they the only information to be considered for placement in school or job-training programs. There is still a dearth of information about how behavior differs between persons with differing IQ scores.[28] For placement in school programs, for medical diagnosis, and for career advising, factors other than IQ must also be part of an individual assessment. The lesson here is that classification systems are necessarily arbitrary and change at the whim of test authors, government bodies, or professional organizations. They are statistical concepts and do not correspond in any real sense to the specific capabilities of any particular person with a given IQ. The classification systems provide descriptive labels that may be useful for communication purposes in a case report or conference, and nothing more.[29] — Alan S. Kaufman and Elizabeth O. Lichtenberger, Assessing Adolescent and Adult Intelligence (2006)

IQ classification tables for current tests [ edit ]

There are a variety of individually administered IQ tests in use in the English-speaking world.[30][31] Not all report test results as "IQ", but most now report a standard score with a median score level of 100. When a test-taker scores higher or lower than the median score, the score is indicated as 15 standard score points higher or lower for each standard deviation difference higher or lower in the test-taker's performance on the test item content.

Wechsler Intelligence Scales [ edit ]

The Wechsler intelligence scales were originally developed from earlier intelligence scales by David Wechsler. The first Wechsler test published was the Wechsler–Bellevue Scale in 1939.[32] The Wechsler IQ tests for children and for adults are the most frequently used individual IQ tests in the English-speaking world[33] and in their translated versions are perhaps the most widely used IQ tests worldwide.[34] The Wechsler tests have long been regarded as the "gold standard" in IQ testing.[35] The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—Fourth Edition (WAIS–IV) was published in 2008 by The Psychological Corporation.[30] The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Fifth Edition (WISC–V) was published in 2014 by The Psychological Corporation, and the Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence—Fourth Edition (WPPSI–IV) was published in 2012 by The Psychological Corporation. Like all current IQ tests, the Wechsler tests report a "deviation IQ" as the standard score for the full-scale IQ, with the norming sample median raw score defined as IQ 100 and a score one standard deviation higher defined as IQ 115 (and one deviation lower defined as IQ 85). Current Wechsler (WAIS–IV, WPPSI–IV) IQ classification IQ Range ("deviation IQ") IQ Classification[36][37] 130 and above Very Superior 120–129 Superior 110–119 High Average 90–109 Average 80–89 Low Average 70–79 Borderline 69 and below Extremely Low Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Fifth Edition (WISC-V) IQ classification IQ Range ("deviation IQ") IQ Classification[38] 130 and above Extremely High 120–129 Very High 110–119 High Average 90–109 Average 80–89 Low Average 70–79 Very Low 69 and below Extremely Low Psychologists have proposed alternative language for Wechsler IQ classifications.[39][40] The term "borderline", which implies being very close to being intellectually disabled, is replaced in the alternative system by a term that doesn't imply a medical diagnosis. Alternate Wechsler IQ Classifications (after Groth-Marnat 2009)[41] Corresponding IQ Range Classifications More value-neutral terms 130+ Very superior Upper extreme 120–129 Superior Well above average 110–119 High average High average 90–109 Average Average 80–89 Low average Low average 70–79 Borderline Well below average 69 and below Extremely low Lower extreme

Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scale Fifth Edition [ edit ]

The current fifth edition of the Stanford–Binet scales (SB5) was developed by Gale H. Roid and published in 2003 by Riverside Publishing.[30] Unlike scoring on previous versions of the Stanford–Binet test, SB5 IQ scoring is deviation scoring in which each standard deviation up or down from the norming sample median score is 15 points from the median score, IQ 100, just like the standard scoring on the Wechsler tests. Stanford–Binet Fifth Edition (SB5) classification[37][42] IQ Range ("deviation IQ") IQ Classification 144+ Very gifted or highly advanced 130–144 Gifted or very advanced 120–129 Superior 110–119 High average 90–109 Average 80–89 Low average 70–79 Borderline impaired or delayed 55–69 Mildly impaired or delayed 40–54 Moderately impaired or delayed

Woodcock–Johnson Test of Cognitive Abilities [ edit ]

The Woodcock–Johnson a III NU Tests of Cognitive Abilities (WJ III NU) was developed by Richard W. Woodcock, Kevin S. McGrew and Nancy Mather and published in 2007 by Riverside.[30] The WJ III classification terms are not applied. Woodcock–Johnson R IQ Score WJ III Classification[43] 131 and above Very superior 121 to 130 Superior 111 to 120 High Average 90 to 110 Average 80 to 89 Low Average 70 to 79 Low 69 and below Very Low

Kaufman Tests [ edit ]

The Kaufman Adolescent and Adult Intelligence Test was developed by Alan S. Kaufman and Nadeen L. Kaufman and published in 1993 by American Guidance Service.[30] Kaufman test scores "are classified in a symmetrical, nonevaluative fashion",[44] in other words the score ranges for classification are just as wide above the median as below the median, and the classification labels do not purport to assess individuals. KAIT 1993 IQ classification 130 and above Upper Extreme 120–129 Well Above Average 110–119 Above average 90–109 Average 80–89 Below Average 70–79 Well Below Average 69 and below Lower Extreme

Who is the best smooth jazz pianist?
Who is the best smooth jazz pianist?

Jeff Lorber is one of the best known smooth jazz players in the industry. He was one of the main pioneers of the post-fusion sound of contemporary...

Read More »
How do you become a singer at 14?
How do you become a singer at 14?

In summary, here's what to do if you're determined to find fame. Work on your vocal technique. Sing and write amazing songs. Become a great...

Read More »

The Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children, Second Edition was developed by Alan S. Kaufman and Nadeen L. Kaufman and published in 2004 by American Guidance Service.[30] KABC-II 2004 Descriptive Categories[45][46] Range of Standard Scores Name of Category 131–160 Upper Extreme 116–130 Above Average 85–115 Average Range 70–84 Below Average 40–69 Lower Extreme

Cognitive Assessment System [ edit ]

The Das-Naglieri Cognitive Assessment System test was developed by Jack Naglieri and J. P. Das and published in 1997 by Riverside.[30] Cognitive Assessment System 1997 full scale score classification[47] Standard Scores Classification 130 and above Very Superior 120–129 Superior 110–119 High Average 90–109 Average 80–89 Low Average 70–79 Below Average 69 and below Well Below Average

Differential Ability Scales [ edit ]

The Differential Ability Scales Second Edition (DAS–II) was developed by Colin D. Elliott and published in 2007 by Psychological Corporation.[30] The DAS-II is a test battery given individually to children, normed for children from ages two years and six months through seventeen years and eleven months.[48] It was normed on 3,480 noninstitutionalized, English-speaking children in that age range.[49] The DAS-II yields a General Conceptual Ability (GCA) score scaled like an IQ score with the median standard score set at 100 and 15 standard score points for each standard deviation up or down from the median. The lowest possible GCA score on DAS–II is 30, and the highest is 170.[50] DAS-II 2007 GCA classification[37][51] GCA General Conceptual Ability Classification ≥ 130 Very high 120–129 High 110–119 Above average 90–109 Average 80–89 Below average 70–79 Low ≤ 69 Very low

Reynolds Intellectual Ability Scales [ edit ]

Reynolds Intellectual Ability Scales (RIAS) were developed by Cecil Reynolds and Randy Kamphaus. The RIAS was published in 2003 by Psychological Assessment Resources.[30] RIAS 2003 Scheme of Verbal Descriptors of Intelligence Test Performance[52] Intelligence test score range Verbal descriptor ≥ 130 Significantly above average 120–129 Moderately above average 110–119 Above average 90–109 Average 80–89 Below average 70–79 Moderately below average ≤ 69 Significantly below average

Historical IQ classification tables [ edit ]

The Psychological Clinic (volume 5 number 1), public domain. Reproduction of an item from the 1908 Binet–Simon intelligence scale, showing three pairs of pictures, about which the tested child was asked, "Which of these two faces is the prettier?" Reproduced from the article "A Practical Guide for Administering the Binet–Simon Scale for Measuring Intelligence" by J. E. Wallace Wallin in the March 1911 issue of the journal(volume 5 number 1), public domain. Lewis Terman, developer of the Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scales, based his English-language Stanford–Binet IQ test on the French-language Binet–Simon test developed by Alfred Binet. Terman believed his test measured the "general intelligence" construct advocated by Charles Spearman (1904).[53][54] Terman differed from Binet in reporting scores on his test in the form of intelligence quotient ("mental age" divided by chronological age) scores after the 1912 suggestion of German psychologist William Stern. Terman chose the category names for score levels on the Stanford–Binet test. When he first chose classification for score levels, he relied partly on the usage of earlier authors who wrote, before the existence of IQ tests, on topics such as individuals unable to care for themselves in independent adult life. Terman's first version of the Stanford–Binet was based on norming samples that included only white, American-born subjects, mostly from California, Nevada, and Oregon.[55] Terman's Stanford–Binet original (1916) classification[56][57] IQ Range ("ratio IQ") IQ Classification Above 140 "Near" genius or genius 120–140 Very superior intelligence 110–120 Superior intelligence 90–110 Normal, or average, intelligence 80–90 Dullness, rarely classifiable as feeble-mindedness 70–80 Border-line deficiency, sometimes classifiable as dullness, often as feeble-mindedness Below 70 Definite feeble-mindedness Rudolph Pintner proposed a set of classification terms in his 1923 book Intelligence Testing: Methods and Results.[4] Pintner commented that psychologists of his era, including Terman, went about "the measurement of an individual's general ability without waiting for an adequate psychological definition."[58] Pintner retained these terms in the 1931 second edition of his book.[59] Albert Julius Levine and Louis Marks proposed a broader set of categories in their 1928 book Testing Intelligence and Achievement.[60][61] Some of the entries came from contemporary terms for people with intellectual disability. Levine and Marks 1928 IQ classification[60][61] IQ Range ("ratio IQ") IQ Classification 175 and over Precocious 150–174 Very superior 125–149 Superior 115–124 Very bright 105–114 Bright 95–104 Average 85–94 Dull 75–84 Borderline 50–74 Morons 25–49 Imbeciles 0–24 Idiots The second revision (1937) of the Stanford–Binet test retained "quotient IQ" scoring, despite earlier criticism of that method of reporting IQ test standard scores.[62] The term "genius" was no longer used for any IQ score range.[63] The second revision was normed only on children and adolescents (no adults), and only "American-born white children".[64] Terman's Stanford–Binet Second Revision (1937) classification[63] IQ Range ("ratio IQ") IQ Classification 140 and over Very superior 120–139 Superior 110–119 High average 90–109 Normal or average 80–89 Low average 70–79 Borderline defective Below 70 Mentally defective A data table published later as part of the manual for the 1960 Third Revision (Form L-M) of the Stanford–Binet test reported score distributions from the 1937 second revision standardization group. Score Distribution of Stanford–Binet 1937 Standardization Group[63] IQ Range ("ratio IQ") Percent of Group 160–169 0.03 150–159 0.2 140–149 1.1 130–139 3.1 120–129 8.2 110–119 18.1 100–109 23.5 90–99 23.0 80–89 14.5 70–79 5.6 60–69 2.0 50–59 0.4 40–49 0.2 30–39 0.03 David Wechsler, developer of the Wechsler–Bellevue Scale of 1939 (which was later developed into the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale) popularized the use of "deviation IQs" as standard scores of IQ tests rather than the "quotient IQs" ("mental age" divided by "chronological age") then used for the Stanford–Binet test.[65] He devoted a whole chapter in his book The Measurement of Adult Intelligence to the topic of IQ classification and proposed different category names from those used by Lewis Terman. Wechsler also criticized the practice of earlier authors who published IQ classification tables without specifying which IQ test was used to obtain the scores reported in the tables.[66] Wechsler–Bellevue 1939 IQ classification IQ Range ("deviation IQ") IQ Classification Percent Included 128 and over Very Superior 2.2 120–127 Superior 6.7 111–119 Bright Normal 16.1 91–110 Average 50.0 80–90 Dull normal 16.1 66–79 Borderline 6.7 65 and below Defective 2.2 In 1958, Wechsler published another edition of his book Measurement and Appraisal of Adult Intelligence. He revised his chapter on the topic of IQ classification and commented that "mental age" scores were not a more valid way to score intelligence tests than IQ scores.[67] He continued to use the same classification terms. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scales 1958 Classification[68] IQ Range ("deviation IQ") IQ Classification (Theoretical) Percent Included 128 and over Very Superior 2.2 120–127 Superior 6.7 111–119 Bright Normal 16.1 91–110 Average 50.0 80–90 Dull normal 16.1 66–79 Borderline 6.7 65 and below Defective 2.2 The third revision (Form L-M) in 1960 of the Stanford–Binet IQ test used the deviation scoring pioneered by David Wechsler. For rough comparability of scores between the second and third revision of the Stanford–Binet test, scoring table author Samuel Pinneau set 100 for the median standard score level and 16 standard score points for each standard deviation above or below that level. The highest score obtainable by direct look-up from the standard scoring tables (based on norms from the 1930s) was IQ 171 at various chronological ages from three years six months (with a test raw score "mental age" of six years and two months) up to age six years and three months (with a test raw score "mental age" of ten years and three months).[69] The classification for Stanford–Binet L-M scores does not include terms such as "exceptionally gifted" and "profoundly gifted" in the test manual itself. David Freides, reviewing the Stanford–Binet Third Revision in 1970 for the Buros Seventh Mental Measurements Yearbook (published in 1972), commented that the test was obsolete by that year.[70] Terman's Stanford–Binet Third Revision (Form L-M) classification[42] IQ Range ("deviation IQ") IQ Classification 140 and over Very superior 120–139 Superior 110–119 High average 90–109 Normal or average 80–89 Low average 70–79 Borderline defective Below 70 Mentally defective The first edition of the Woodcock–Johnson Tests of Cognitive Abilities was published by Riverside in 1977. The classifications used by the WJ-R Cog were "modern in that they describe levels of performance as opposed to offering a diagnosis."[43] Woodcock–Johnson R IQ Score WJ-R Cog 1977 Classification[43] 131 and above Very superior 121 to 130 Superior 111 to 120 High Average 90 to 110 Average 80 to 89 Low Average 70 to 79 Low 69 and below Very Low The revised version of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (the WAIS-R) was developed by David Wechsler and published by Psychological Corporation in 1981. Wechsler changed a few of the boundaries for classification categories and a few of their names compared to the 1958 version of the test. The test's manual included information about how the actual percentage of persons in the norming sample scoring at various levels compared to theoretical expectations. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scales 1981 Classification[71] IQ Range ("deviation IQ") IQ Classification Actual Percent Included Theoretical Percent Included 130+ Very Superior 2.6 2.2 120–129 Superior 6.9 6.7 110–119 High Average 16.6 16.1 90–109 Average 49.1 50.0 80–89 Low Average 16.1 16.1 70–79 Borderline 6.4 6.7 below 70 Mentally Retarded 2.3 2.2 The Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC) was developed by Alan S. Kaufman and Nadeen L. Kaufman and published in 1983 by American Guidance Service. K-ABC 1983 Ability Classifications[71] Range of Standard Scores Name of Category Percent of Norm Sample Theoretical Percent Included 130+ Upper Extreme 2.3 2.2 120–129 Well Above Average 7.4 6.7 110–119 Above Average 16.7 16.1 90–109 Average 49.5 50.0 80–89 Below Average 16.1 16.1 70–79 Well Below Average 6.1 6.7 below 70 Lower Extreme 2.1 2.2 The fourth revision of the Stanford–Binet scales (S-B IV) was developed by Thorndike, Hagen, and Sattler and published by Riverside Publishing in 1986. It retained the deviation scoring of the third revision with each standard deviation from the median being defined as a 16 IQ point difference. The S-B IV adopted new classification terminology. After this test was published, psychologist Nathan Brody lamented that IQ tests had still not caught up with advances in research on human intelligence during the twentieth century.[72] Stanford–Binet Intelligence Scale, Fourth Edition (S-B IV) 1986 classification[73][74] IQ Range ("deviation IQ") IQ Classification 132 and above Very superior 121–131 Superior 111–120 High average 89–110 Average 79–88 Low average 68–78 Slow learner 67 or below Mentally retarded

Do birds have a sixth sense?
Do birds have a sixth sense?

Pigeons have a sixth sense called magnetoreception. Many migratory animals, from salmon to sea turtles, have a unique ability to detect Earth's...

Read More »
Why do guitarists play in drop D?
Why do guitarists play in drop D?

Tuning to Drop D makes it easier to shift your guitar to a range that makes it easier for singers with lower voices to hit the correct notes as you...

Read More »

Wechsler (WAIS–III) 1997 IQ test classification IQ Range ("deviation IQ") IQ Classification 130 and above Very superior 120–129 Superior 110–119 High average 90–109 Average 80–89 Low average 70–79 Borderline 69 and below Extremely low

Classification of low IQ [ edit ]

The earliest terms for classifying individuals of low intelligence were medical or legal terms that preceded the development of IQ testing.[10][11] The legal system recognized a concept of some individuals being so cognitively impaired that they were not responsible for criminal behavior. Medical doctors sometimes encountered adult patients who could not live independently, being unable to take care of their own daily living needs. Various terms were used to attempt to classify individuals with varying degrees of intellectual disability. Many of the earliest terms are now considered extremely offensive. In current medical diagnosis, IQ scores alone are not conclusive for a finding of intellectual disability. Recently adopted diagnostic standards place the major emphasis on the adaptive behavior of each individual, with IQ score just being one factor in diagnosis in addition to adaptive behavior scales, and no category of intellectual disability being defined primarily by IQ scores.[75] Psychologists point out that evidence from IQ testing should always be used with other assessment evidence in mind: "In the end, any and all interpretations of test performance gain diagnostic meaning when they are corroborated by other data sources and when they are empirically or logically related to the area or areas of difficulty specified in the referral."[76] In the United States, the Supreme Court ruled in the case Atkins v. Virginia, 536 U.S. 304 (2002) that states could not impose capital punishment on persons with "mental retardation", defined in subsequent cases as persons with IQ scores below 70.[citation needed] This legal standard continues to be actively litigated in capital cases.[77]

Historical [ edit ]

Historically, terms for intellectual disability eventually become perceived as an insult, in a process commonly known as the euphemism treadmill.[78][79][80] The terms mental retardation and mentally retarded became popular in the middle of the 20th century to replace the previous set of terms, which included "imbecile", "idiot", "feeble-minded", and "moron",[81] among others, and are now considered offensive, often extremely so. By the end of the 20th century, retardation and retard became widely seen as disparaging and politically incorrect, although they are still used in some clinical contexts.[82] The American Association for the Study of the Feeble-minded divided adults with intellectual deficits into three categories. Idiot indicated the greatest degree of intellectual disability in which a person's mental age is below three years. Imbecile indicated an intellectual disability less severe than idiocy and a mental age between three and seven years. Moron was defined as someone a mental age between eight and twelve.[83] Alternative definitions of these terms based on IQ were also used.[citation needed] The term cretin dates to 1770–80 and comes from a dialectal French word for Christian.[84] The implication was that people with significant intellectual or developmental disabilities were "still human" (or "still Christian") and deserved to be treated with basic human dignity. Although cretin is no longer in use, the term cretinism is still used to refer to the mental and physical disability resulting from untreated congenital hypothyroidism.[84] Mongolism and Mongoloid idiot were terms used to identify someone with Down syndrome, as the doctor who first described the syndrome, John Langdon Down, believed that children with Down syndrome shared facial similarities with the now-obsolete category of "Mongolian race". The Mongolian People's Republic requested that the medical community cease the use of the term; in 1960, the World Health Organization agreed the term should cease being used.[85] Retarded comes from the Latin retardare, "to make slow, delay, keep back, or hinder", so mental retardation meant the same as mentally delayed. The first record of retarded in relation to being mentally slow was in 1895. The term mentally retarded was used to replace terms like idiot, moron, and imbecile because retarded was not then a derogatory term. By the 1960s, however, the term had taken on a partially derogatory meaning. The noun retard is particularly seen as pejorative; a BBC survey in 2003 ranked it as the most offensive disability-related word.[86] The terms mentally retarded and mental retardation are still fairly common, but organizations such as the Special Olympics and Best Buddies are striving to eliminate their use and often refer to retard and its variants as the "r-word". These efforts resulted in U.S. federal legislation, known as Rosa's Law, which replaced the term mentally retarded with the term intellectual disability in federal law.[87][88]

Classification of high IQ [ edit ]

Genius [ edit ]

Galton in his later years

Francis Galton (1822–1911) was a pioneer in investigating both eminent human achievement and mental testing. In his book Hereditary Genius, writing before the development of IQ testing, he proposed that hereditary influences on eminent achievement are strong, and that eminence is rare in the general population. Lewis Terman chose "'near' genius or genius" as the classification label for the highest classification on his 1916 version of the Stanford–Binet test.[56] By 1926, Terman began publishing about a longitudinal study of California schoolchildren who were referred for IQ testing by their schoolteachers, called Genetic Studies of Genius, which he conducted for the rest of his life. Catherine M. Cox, a colleague of Terman's, wrote a whole book, The Early Mental Traits of 300 Geniuses, published as volume 2 of The Genetic Studies of Genius book series, in which she analyzed biographical data about historic geniuses. Although her estimates of childhood IQ scores of historical figures who never took IQ tests have been criticized on methodological grounds,[89][90][91] Cox's study was thorough in finding out what else matters besides IQ in becoming a genius.[92] By the 1937 second revision of the Stanford–Binet test, Terman no longer used the term "genius" as an IQ classification, nor has any subsequent IQ test.[63][93] In 1939, Wechsler wrote "we are rather hesitant about calling a person a genius on the basis of a single intelligence test score."[94] The Terman longitudinal study in California eventually provided historical evidence on how genius is related to IQ scores.[95] Many California pupils were recommended for the study by schoolteachers. Two pupils who were tested but rejected for inclusion in the study because of IQ scores too low for the study grew up to be Nobel Prize winners in physics: William Shockley[96][97] and Luis Walter Alvarez.[98][99] Based on the historical findings of the Terman study and on biographical examples such as Richard Feynman, who had an IQ of 125 and went on to win the Nobel Prize in physics and become widely known as a genius,[100][101] the current view of psychologists and other scholars of genius is that a minimum IQ, about 125, is strictly necessary for genius, but that IQ is sufficient for the development of genius only when combined with the other influences identified by Cox's biographical study: an opportunity for talent development along with the characteristics of drive and persistence. Charles Spearman, bearing in mind the influential theory that he originated—that intelligence comprises both a "general factor" and "special factors" more specific to particular mental tasks—, wrote in 1927, "Every normal man, woman, and child is, then, a genius at something, as well as an idiot at something."[102]

Giftedness [ edit ]

A major point of consensus among all scholars of intellectual giftedness is that there is no generally agreed upon definition of giftedness.[103] Although there is no scholarly agreement about identifying gifted learners, there is a de facto reliance on IQ scores for identifying participants in school gifted education programs. In practice, many school districts in the United States use an IQ score of 130, including roughly the upper 2 to 3 percent of the national population as a cut-off score for inclusion in school gifted programs.[104] Five levels of giftedness have been suggested to differentiate the vast difference in abilities that exists between children on varying ends of the gifted spectrum.[105] Although there is no strong consensus on the validity of these quantifiers, they are accepted by many experts of gifted children. Levels of Giftedness (M.U. Gross)[105] Classification IQ Range σ Prevalence Mildly gifted 115–129 +1.00–+1.99 1:6 Moderately gifted 130–144 +2.00–+2.99 1:44 Highly gifted 145–159 +3.00–+3.99 1:1,000 Exceptionally gifted 160–179 +4.00–+5.33 1:10,000 Profoundly gifted 180– +5.33– < 1:1,000,000 As long ago as 1937, Lewis Terman pointed out that error of estimation in IQ scoring increases as IQ score increases, so that there is less and less certainty about assigning a test-taker to one band of scores or another as one looks at higher bands.[106] Current IQ tests also have large error bands for high IQ scores.[107] As an underlying reality, such distinctions as those between "exceptionally gifted" and "profoundly gifted" have never been well established. All longitudinal studies of IQ have shown that test-takers can bounce up and down in score, and thus switch up and down in rank order as compared to one another, over the course of childhood. Some test-givers claim that IQ classification categories such as "profoundly gifted" are meaningful, but those are based on the obsolete Stanford–Binet Third Revision (Form L-M) test.[108] The highest reported standard score for most IQ tests is IQ 160, approximately the 99.997th percentile (leaving aside the issue of the considerable error in measurement at that level of IQ on any IQ test).[109] IQ scores above this level are dubious as there are insufficient normative cases upon which to base a statistically justified rank-ordering.[110][111] Moreover, there has never been any validation of the Stanford–Binet L-M on adult populations, and there is no trace of such terminology in the writings of Lewis Terman. Although two current tests attempt to provide "extended norms" that allow for classification of different levels of giftedness, those norms are not based on well validated data.[112]

References [ edit ]

Bibliography [ edit ]

Why do I play piano better at night?
Why do I play piano better at night?

You get better after a night of sleep, study finds. According to researchers at the University of Montreal, the regions of the brain below the...

Read More »
Does learning piano help with math?
Does learning piano help with math?

Playing the piano improves your mathematical ability. Notes and rhythms, as well as music theory, are based on math. Reading music and counting...

Read More »
Is a half hour guitar lesson enough?
Is a half hour guitar lesson enough?

For adult learners who have significant experience in playing music, it begins to make sense to think about investing more than a half-hour for a...

Read More »
How much does it cost to fix a broken key on a keyboard?
How much does it cost to fix a broken key on a keyboard?

Many laptop repair professionals charge over $100 just because you have a damaged key. We provide a solution for under $5. Laptop-keys.com is a...

Read More »